文章詳目資料

教育心理學報 ScopusTSSCI

  • 加入收藏
  • 下載文章
篇名 國小二、三年級學童朗讀流暢能力之成長模式探究
卷期 53:2
並列篇名 Growth Model of Oral Reading Fluency Among Second and Third Graders
作者 張郁雯
頁次 463-480
關鍵字 朗讀流暢能力分段成長模式成長斜率標準文本oral reading fluencypiecewise growth modelgrowth ratestandard passagesTSSCIScopus
出刊日期 202112
DOI 10.6251/BEP.202112_53(2).0009

中文摘要

朗讀流暢性測驗常用於設定學童的成長目標以及監控其學習進展。先前以課文為測試文本的研究指出朗讀流暢能力的成長符合線性模式。本研究以標準文本探討國小二、三年級學童朗讀流暢能力的成長模式並估計其成長率。研究參與者為新北市兩所小學的二年級學生57名,三年級學生51名。每三週個別施測一次朗讀流暢測驗,一個學年共施測13次。以階層模式分析資料,結果發現分段成長模式比線性成長模式與資料的適配度較好,意即學童在同年度的上、下學期有不同的成長速度。二上、二下、三上以及三下每個學期可以進步的字數分別為13、18、20和6個字。二年級上學期初,每分鐘朗讀的正確字數平均為109個字;三年級上學期初約為137個字,下學期期末平均為165個字。二年級學童在學期初的朗讀流暢能力與其成長率之間沒有顯著之相關。然而,在三年級上學期,朗讀流暢能力與其成長率相關值為0.32達顯著水準。下學期成長率與朗讀流暢初始能力卻呈現顯著的負相關。最後,作者根據研究結果對朗讀流暢性研究與教學實務之意涵提出討論。

英文摘要

Fluent reading is an indication of proficient decoding and comprehension. Studies have demonstrated that oral reading fluency (ORF) correlates positively with reading comprehension. The ORF assessment comprises a set of individually administered 1-minute reading probes. These tests are designed to monitor learning progress toward predetermined instructional goals and identify children who may require additional instructional support. Although many studies have explored Mandarin ORF performance levels at each grade, the reading probes used are generally adopted from textbooks. However, results have indicated that the curriculum-based ORF is 50 characters per minute more than that measured by the standard probe assessment. This suggests that reading fluency levels are affected by the equivalence of the reading probes. This study is the first to use standard probes to investigate the developmental and expected growth of Mandarin ORF in the second and third grades of elementary schools. The number of correct words read per minute (the level) and the growth rate of student reading fluency are two key indicators of reading progress. However, few studies have investigated the growth rate of Mandarin ORF. Early studies tended to assert that within-year ORF growth is linear rather than quadratic, but recent research has indicated that growth rates differ within a school year. This study examined the growth model of reading fluency and estimated its growth rate. Students with a large vocabulary tend to have a higher ORF level. According to the Matthew effect, students with higher ORF levels have a greater growth rate. Previous Mandarin ORF research does not support this inference. Furthermore, the correlation between initial ORF levels and growth rates has not been reported. However, this issue is important; if students with different ORF levels have different growth rates, then appropriate learning growth goals should be set based on their initial abilities when planning interventions. This study tracked the development of ORF skills over the course of one academic year to investigate the following research questions: (1) What are the growth models of ORF for second- and third-grade students in elementary schools? (2) What is the growth rate of ORF over the semesters for second- and third-grade students? (3) Is the growth rate for reading fluency related to the ORF level at the beginning of the semester? The participants comprised 57 second-grade and 51 third-grade students and 53 girls and 55 boys. The students underwent an ORF test (Chang, 2018). For each grade, 19 reading probes, with a text length of 350–400 characters, were used. The alternate-form reliability was between .80 and .89. The generalizability coefficients for the second- and third-grade conditions were approximately .84 and .85, respectively, when one reading probe was used to estimate ORF performance. When three reading probes were used, the generalizability coefficients were .94 and .95, respectively. The correlation coefficients between ORF score and reading comprehension test score were .60 and .67, respectively. The ORF tests were administered at 3-week intervals during each semester in the 2012 academic year. Each student took ORF tests at 13 time points, with three reading probes administered at the beginning of the school year (pretest), at the end of the first semester (midterm test), and at the end of the second semester (posttest). The average score across the three reading probes was calculated to obtain reliable growth rates. At each of the other time points, a single ORF score was collected. Eight examiners were trained in the administration and scoring of the ORF tests in a 4-hour workshop, during which the assessment and scoring procedures were introduced and the examiners practiced the procedures in pairs to ensure they could conduct the procedure reliably. Overall interrater reliability was .99 for this study. A hierarchical linear model was used to assess the goodness of fit between the data and five models: the null model (M0), random intercept model (M1), random coefficient model (M2), piecewise growth model (M3), and quadratic growth model (M4). The intraclass correlation (ICC) of the M0 model for the second grade was .73, meaning that 73% of the difference in reading fluency scores originated in differences between students (level 2) and 27% of the variance originated from differences within individuals (level 1). The ICC for the third grade was .81. The results suggested high heterogeneity between students and the necessity of a hierarchical model. Among five models, a piecewise growth model had the lowest deviance value and thus the best fit with the data. Thus, the children had different growth rates in the two semesters within the same academic year. On average, the number of characters the second and third graders could read correctly per minute was 109 and 137 at the beginning of the academic year, respectively. At the end of the third grade, the mean ORF was 165 characters. In the fall, the second-grade ORF growth averaged 13 characters per semester, whereas the average was approximately 18 characters per semester in the spring. The third graders’ mean growth rates were 20 and 6 characters in the first and second semesters, respectively. The second-grade growth rates in the two semesters were similar, but ORF gains were more modest during the second half of the academic year for the third grade. The correlation between ORF and the growth rate for the second-grade students was not significant. However, in the first semester of the third grade, the correlation between ORF and growth rate was significant and had a correlation coefficient of .32 . In the second semester, the correlation between ORF and growth rate was -.43. In different grades and semesters, the relationships between initial ORF and growth rate varied. This finding reminds teachers that they should consider the students’ grades and initial ORF when setting teaching goals. The results of this study established the expected ORF level and growth rate for second- and third-grade students, enabling teachers to identify high-risk students that are falling behind in reading to determine the effects of intervention by examining their change in growth rate and adjust their instruction accordingly. Knowing student’s ORF scores may help teachers plan reading instruction that is more responsive to the individual. By collecting multiple assessment scores, educators and researchers no longer must assume linear developmental trends because nonlinear functions can be fit to the time-series data. The nonlinear growth pattern suggests that instructional goals derived from a linear change model may require revision and that student growth expectations may require modification at different points in the academic year. The results only partially support the Matthew effect. At the beginning of the semester, the growth rate of the second-year students with strong ORF was not significantly greater than that of those with weaker ORF. However, in the first semester of the third grade, the level of ORF and the growth rate was significantly and positively correlated. Participants with strong reading fluency grew faster in their reading ability during the fall semester than those with weaker ORF. Therefore, the results may enable educators to recognize the need for more intensive instructional support during periods when rapid growth is expected, which is the second semester of the second grade through the first semester of third grade.

相關文獻